For many, the ability to contort their tongue into various shapes seems like a simple party trick, yet the underlying mechanisms and prevalence of these oral feats are surprisingly complex. From forming a "U" shape to the incredibly rare cloverleaf, these tongue movements have long fascinated both the public and the scientific community.
Common tongue contortions include:
Lifting both sides of the tongue to form a "U" shape.
Twisting the tongue upside down to the left or right.
Folding the tongue either over or under itself.
Creating a "cloverleaf" or "trefoil" tongue.
For decades, a common misconception persisted that the ability to roll one's tongue was a genetic trait. This notion largely stemmed from a flawed 1940 study by scientist Alfred Sturtevant. His research, which categorized some children as "unable to roll their tongues" merely because they refused to open their mouths, and noted instances where children could roll their tongues despite their parents being unable to, ultimately failed to support a genetic link. Subsequent studies have consistently disproved Sturtevant’s initial findings, indicating that genetics do not determine tongue-rolling ability.
Among the various tongue tricks, the cloverleaf tongue stands out as a particularly rare and impressive feat. While a significant portion of the population can roll their tongue into a "U" shape, the ability to form a cloverleaf is far less common. According to a study published in the journal Dysphagia, a remarkable 83.7% of the population demonstrated the ability to roll their tongue. However, the study also revealed that only a small, elite group—just 14.7% of the population—could achieve the complex cloverleaf tongue. This makes the cloverleaf a truly distinctive talent.
So, if genetics aren't the primary factor, what enables these intricate tongue movements? Research suggests that language plays a crucial role. A study published in the Journal of Clinical & Diagnostic Research investigated 450 medical students in Malaysia from three distinct ethnic groups—Malay, Malaysian Chinese, and Malaysian Indian—who spoke various languages. The findings indicated that Malaysian Indians exhibited greater versatility in tongue movements compared to the Malay and Malaysian Chinese groups. The study concluded that the capacity for certain tongue shapes and movements is influenced by the "lingual demands" of an individual’s native language. This suggests that speaking a language that requires more dynamic tongue movements could enhance one's ability to learn and perform complex tongue tricks.
Therefore, if you're looking to master tongue-twisting, rolling, or even the elusive cloverleaf, consistent practice is key. And if your native language involves a wide range of tongue movements, you might just have a natural advantage!
Pro Tip
The content of the article is shared by netizens, please carefully identify it